- Focus
- Open access
- Published:
Who's afraid of Homo sapiens?
Journal of Biomedical Discovery and Collaboration volume 1, Article number: 17 (2006)
Abstract
Understanding how humans differ from other animals, as well as how we are like them, requires comparative investigations. For the purpose of documenting the distinctive features of humans, the most informative research involves comparing humans to our closest relatives–the chimpanzees and other great apes. Psychology and anthropology have maintained a tradition of empirical comparative research on human specializations of cognition. The neurosciences, by contrast, have been dominated by the model-animal research paradigm, which presupposes the commonality of "basic" features of brain organization across species and discourages serious treatment of species differences. As a result, the neurosciences have made little progress in understanding human brain specializations. Recent developments in neuroimaging, genomics, and other non-invasive techniques make it possible to directly compare humans and nonhuman species at levels of organization that were previously inaccessible, offering the hope of gaining a better understanding of the species-specific features of the human brain. This hope will be dashed, however, if chimpanzees and other great ape species become unavailable for even non-invasive research.
Introduction
What a pleasure it is to read Karl Pribram's 1970 James Arthur Lecture on human specializations of the brain and cognition [1]. It takes us back to a time of bold scientific conjectures about human nature, an era that saw the publication of Konrad Lorenz's On Aggression (1966; first German edition, 1963) [2], B.F. Skinner's Beyond Freedom and Dignity (1971) [3], and E.O. Wilson's Sociobiology (1975) [4], to take a few examples, all books that intrigued me (and sometimes infuriated me) as a student. Pribram displays an impressively broad knowledge of psychology and neuroscience, achieving a synthesis that few scientists at the time would have been capable of and that even fewer scientists could credibly attempt today. The work is also impressive because the author is reasonably specific about what he considers to be the distinctive characteristics of human beings. In the realm of cognition, he emphasizes meaning, and in particular the ability of humans to construct propositions. He doesn't quite claim that language is a signature specialization of human cognition, but this is implicit in his argument. He considers, for example, that chimpanzees can use signs, but is not impressed with their syntactic capacity. In the realm of neuroscience, Pribram is less specific, but he does suggest that the distinctiveness of the human brain involves not the organization of cortico-cortical connections, as one might suppose, but rather the organization of cortical connections with subcortical motor structures, consistent with his view that meaning is grounded in intention and action.
How do Pribram's conjectures hold up in the light of modern research? It must be acknowledged that Pribram's lecture did not spark a new set of investigations into human cognitive or neurological specializations. In the 1970s, comparative human-ape research was dominated by the ape-language project, which involved teaching apes (mainly chimpanzees) to communicate using systems of manual gesture, such as American Sign Language, or with physical tokens. Popular culture today takes it as given that apes share with humans the capacity for language. Students of language, however, have for the most part drawn the opposite lesson from the ape-language project, concluding that apes demonstrate little productive language capacity, even after intensive training, and that language is a human specialization (e.g., [5–12]). Recent years have brought new ideas about human cognitive specializations. For example, Povinelli [13–15] has proposed that unlike humans, chimpanzees do not form explicit representations of abstract, unobservable variables, such as mental states (in the case of behavioral causation) or mass force (in the case of physical interactions). These proposals have prompted much debate in the comparative cognition community, along with new experiments (to get the flavor of the debate, see Tomasello et al. [16] and accompanying commentaries). It is noteworthy that Povinelli's "reinterpretation" hypothesis [14, 17, 18], which holds that humans create new causal interpretations of ancient behaviors, recalls Pribram's claim that humans create and represent meaning through propositions. Additionally, anthropologists have revived the classical idea that culture is a human adaptation, and have begun to characterize cognitive specializations involved in the acquisition and transmission of cultural knowledge (e.g., [19–21]).
What are the evolutionary specializations of the human brain?
While a thin but vital thread of research on human cognitive specializations runs through psychology and anthropology from the 1970s to the present, in the neurosciences the thread has been drawn so fine as to be nearly invisible. Consider Pribram's list of human brain specializations: brain enlargement, hemispheric dominance and specialization, and "somewhat more generalized" cytoarchitecture. Of these, brain enlargement (encephalization) remains well accepted, although the nature of this enlargement–was there a general enlargement or a more specific enlargement of association cortex?–is controversial [22–26]. Whether humans show greater hemispheric specialization than do apes and other nonhuman primates is also controversial: there is some evidence that our closest relatives, the chimpanzees and other apes, possess homologues of Wernicke's and Broca's areas, and that these are lateralized in ways that are at least qualitatively similar to humans [27–30]. There are, however, those who maintain that humans show extreme hemispheric asymmetry and functional laterality, and that these are among the defining features of humans [31–33].
A few human specializations have been identified at finer levels of structural organization. For example, the histological organization of human primary visual cortex displays some striking differences with apes and monkeys [34]. Humans have several populations of morphologically and biochemically distinctive pyramidal cells in anterior cortex [35–37]. The spacing and cell density of minicolumns differ in Wernicke's area of the left and right hemispheres in humans, but not in chimpanzees or monkeys [38]. With the extension of functional imaging techniques to nonhuman primates has come evidence that the functions of homologous visual areas differ to some extent between humans and macaque monkeys (the animals most commonly used as models of the human visual system), and also raise the possibility that humans have several higher-order visual areas not present in monkeys [39–41]. Unfortunately, there have not yet been functional imaging studies of ape visual cortex, so it is not possible to say whether these differences represent true human specializations or specializations of the larger group (the Hominoidea) to which both humans and apes belong; the possibility that differences represent specializations of macaques also needs to be considered. One can point to additional differences between humans and various "model" species, such as human-macaque or human-mouse differences–some of them quite remarkable [42]–but as interesting and important as these differences undoubtedly are, we cannot safely conclude that these are human specializations without studies that directly compare humans to apes, as well as to other species.
What slim pickings! It seems extraordinary that neuroscience has so little to offer on a matter so fundamental as what is it about our brains that makes us human. No less disturbing is the understanding that if we don't have the information to say much about how our brains differ from those of other animals, we probably know less than we suppose about how humans resemble other animals, also.
Why do we know so little about human brain specializations?
How is it that after more than 125 years of experimental neuroscience we know so little about how the human brain differs from that of other species? Consider the kind of research required to understand the place of humans among animals: one would need studies that examine human brain structure and function in detail, and that compare humans to other species using comparable techniques. The most informative comparisons, if one's goal is to understand human specializations, are with the great apes, and especially with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos ("pygmy" chimpanzees; Pan paniscus), as these are the animals most closely related to us. If one's main goal is to understand what humans share with other animals, comparative studies would still be essential, although in this case one would need to study a broad range of species, of varying degrees of relationship to humans.
We do not have these kinds of studies. The obvious reason for this is it has been difficult to study the human brain directly in much detail. Historically, the most valuable investigative techniques in the neurosciences have required invasive and terminal techniques, or other manipulations (such as genetic manipulations) that we regard as unethical in humans. So, for the most part, neuroscientists study not real humans but surrogate humans, nonhuman species that we believe are similar enough to humans to be informative about the human condition, but not so similar as to be ethically problematic (or at least not prohibitively so). This approach–the model-animal paradigm–has been widely employed in the experimental biomedical sciences, particularly, in preference to the broader, comparative approach that has flourished in some other biological disciplines.
The adoption of the model-animal paradigm brought about important changes in the way scientists understand the role of animals in research [43–46]. In the early part of the 20th century, before the establishment of currently favored model-animal species such as rats, mice, and rhesus monkeys, experimental biologists sought to identify features of biological organization that are shared by a wide variety of species. To identify these features, it was felt necessary to actually compare a wide variety of species. Attitudes changed as the favored model animals became entrenched as research resources: increasingly it was assumed that the experimentally identified characteristics of these species have broad generality and the imperative to empirically demonstrate generality faded. As historians of science have observed, the establishment of breeding colonies of rodents transformed these animals from subjects of research into something more like standardized chemical reagents [43, 44, 47]. From this perspective, cross-species variation in biological organization is a problem, a potential threat to the standing of one's particular model animal within the larger research community. Species differences, if they are acknowledged at all, tend to be soft-peddled and relegated to the status of noise: it's the commonalities that matter. In this context, the word "basic" has come to mean "common" or "widely shared." As a result, "basic" keeps close company with "same," so that one often hears the expression "the same basic X," where X can be structure, function, organization, or any other attribute of organisms.
This analysis does not imply that the use of nonhuman species as research models of human biological systems or specific diseases is misguided; after all, humans do share many features in common with other animals. What is problematic is the mindset fostered by the model-animal paradigm: viewing the biological world primarily through this lens has serious and negative consequences for the scientific enterprise. For one thing, because the model-animal paradigm discourages systematic, rigorous treatment of the similarities and differences across species, we remain in the position of assuming generality, rather than demonstrating it empirically. This is a particular problem for the neurosciences–mammalian cerebral cortex, for example, has proven to be far more variable across species than believed a decade or two ago, fundamentally compromising the idea that there is a "basic uniformity" of cortical design [48–51]. In addition, by failing to take differences seriously, we have largely ignored the correlated variations between biological organization and function produced by evolution, and in so doing have ignored a very rich source of information about structure-function relationships (for examples of how such variations can be exploited, see [52, 53]). Finally, the model-animal paradigm has little room for human specializations, for if we consider to be "basic" only those characteristics of biological organization that are shared among species, then the features that distinguish human brains from those of other species don't count as "basic." These would include some of the most interesting and important things we would like to know about human beings, such as what features of our brains support our distinctive cognitive capacities and what features render humans (alone among primates) vulnerable to Alzheimer's disease [54, 55].
Making room for humans: the critical need for human-chimpanzee comparisons
We can begin to redress some of the deficiencies in our scientific knowledge stemming from the primacy of the model-animal paradigm, with its very indirect approach to understanding human nature, by investing more in comparative studies. The time is especially propitious for studies that address human specializations through direct comparisons of humans and closely related species, because new techniques are available that make it possible to directly study the human brain in great detail, and in ethically acceptable ways, and we can apply these same techniques to the study of our closest relatives.
As the spatial resolution of neuroimaging techniques has improved, it has become practical to use them to compare the structure and function of human brains to those of other primates. While comparisons with macaque monkeys–the favored model nonhuman primates–have been emphasized [56–59], these approaches can be applied to the study of chimpanzees and other ape species as well [60, 61], opening the door to new explorations of human-specific brain organization. In addition, histological studies of humans, using tissue obtained postmortem, have undergone something of a rebirth, driven in part by the need to provide a better understanding of the regional organization of human cortex for the correct interpretation of functional imaging results than can be obtained from the hundred-year-old cortical map of Brodmann [62]. It is clear that one can obtain reliable and informative results with postmortem tissue derived not only from humans, as well as from apes and other nonhuman primates (e.g., [34–36, 63, 64]).
The growth of knowledge about the genomic organization of humans and nonhuman primates, including chimpanzees, creates additional opportunities for understanding human-specific brain organization. A number of studies have identified gene-expression or gene-sequence differences between humans, chimpanzees, and other nonhuman primates (reviewed in [65–67]). Large-scale genomic changes in human evolution have also been documented, involving duplications and rearrangements of DNA, which in some cases have even resulted in the creation of novel, human-specific genes (e.g., [68–70]).
Comparative genomics research on human-ape differences has generated enormous interest in the popular media, which is not surprising given the strong interest of the public in understanding what makes us human. One important point that is not widely appreciated, however, is that the identification of genetic differences that distinguish humans from other animals is ultimately of little value if we cannot connect the genetic differences to phenotypic differences [71, 72]. At the present time, the wealth of information about human-chimpanzee genetic differences stands in stark contrast to the poverty of our understanding of human-chimpanzee differences in brain organization. My colleagues and I have suggested that we can use the information from comparative genetic studies as clues to identify previously unknown phenotypic specializations of the human brain, for example, by following the trail from genes to mRNA and protein expression in tissue [66, 73]. While we have focused on comparative histological studies, genomics-driven "phenotype discovery" could employ any of the growing array of non-invasive, non-terminal techniques (biochemical, proteomic, imaging, behavioral) we have for comparing humans to other animals.
Will there be chimpanzees to study?
The advent of technologies like genomics and neuroimaging gives me hope that neuroscience can adopt a more direct approach to the study of human nature and once again deal with fundamental questions of the kind addressed in Karl Pribram's lecture. My optimism is tempered, however, by the knowledge that we might soon lose a resource vital for this pursuit. Understanding what makes our brain distinctively human requires comparing humans to chimpanzees, our closest relatives. In a world where the value of animals in research is measured mainly in terms of their utility as "models," chimpanzees don't fit in very well, as they are more expensive and difficult to maintain than, say, rodents, and as with humans, their use in invasive research is restricted. So, despite the obvious value of comparative studies of chimpanzees and humans for understanding the human brain (including its distinctive vulnerability to neurodegenerative disease), and despite the availability of powerful new techniques for comparing human and chimpanzee brains, it may soon become impossible to pursue this essential research. In 1997, faced with a larger population of captive chimpanzees than could reasonably be supported, NIH imposed a moratorium on breeding chimpanzees. Consequently, the population is not being replaced as animals die of old age and the number of chimpanzees available for the kind of non-invasive research described above–the kind of research we conduct with humans–is rapidly shrinking. Given the highly endangered status of chimpanzees, which seem destined for extinction in the wild within a few decades, it is unlikely that such a resource, once lost, would be reconstituted. In 2007, NIH will decide whether or not to lift the moratorium. As someone committed to understanding the structure, function, and diseases of the human nervous system, I hope the moratorium will be ended. It seems prudent, now that the population has been reduced, to allow enough breeding to maintain a viable population. To be sure, it would be simpler to continue the current policy, avoiding the political heat that would result from choosing to maintain the chimpanzee population, and instead devote our limited resources to supporting mice and a few other favored model-animal species. But, after all, the mouse we will always have with us, whereas chimpanzees will be with us for only a short time longer, if we continue on our current course. If we fail to preserve the means to understand what makes us human, in health and in disease, future generations will surely ask: What were they thinking?
References
Pribram KH: What Makes Man Human?. 1971, New York: American Museum of Natural History
Lorenz K: On Aggression, [originally published as Das Sogennate Böse: Zur Naturgeshichte der Aggression, Dr. G. Borotha-Schoeler Verlag, Vienna, 1963]. 1966, New York: Harcourt
Skinner BF: Beyond Freedom and Dignity. 1971, New York: Knopf
Wilson EO: Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. 1975, Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press
Bickerton D: Language & Species. 1990, Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Pinker S: The Language Instinct. 1994, New York, NY: W. Morrow and Co., 1
Rivas E: Recent use of signs by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in interactions with humans. J Comp Psychol. 2005, 119 (4): 404-417. 10.1037/0735-7036.119.4.404.
Seidenberg MS, Petitto LA: Signing behavior in apes: A critical review. Cognition. 1979, 7 (2): 177-215. 10.1016/0010-0277(79)90019-2.
Wallman J: Aping Language. 1992, New York: Cambridge University Press
Deacon TW: The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. 1997, New York; London: W.W. Norton, 1
Lieberman P: The Biology and Evolution of Language. 1984, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press
Tomasello M, Call J: Primate Cognition. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press
Povinelli DJ: Reconstructing the evolution of mind. American Psychologist. 1993, 48 (5): 493-509. 10.1037/0003-066X.48.5.493.
Povinelli DJ: Folk Physics for Apes: The Chimpanzee's Theory of How the World Works. 2000, Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press
Povinelli DJ, Eddy TJ, Hobson RP, Tomasello M: What young chimpanzees know about seeing. 1996, Chicago, I11.: Society for Research in Child Development
Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H: Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005, 28 (5): 675-10.1017/S0140525X05000129.
Povinelli DJ, Barth J: Reinterpreting behavior: a human specialization?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005, 28 (5): 712-713. 10.1017/S0140525X05450128.
Povinelli DJ, Bering JM: The mentality of apes revisited. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002, 11 (4): 115-119. 10.1111/1467-8721.00181.
Barkow JH, Cosmides L, Tooby J: The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. 1992, New York ; Oxford: Oxford University Press
Henrich J, McElreath R: The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2003, 12 (3): 123-135. 10.1002/evan.10110.
Richerson PJ, Boyd R: Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. 2005, Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Passingham RE: The frontal cortex: does size matter?. Nat Neurosci. 2002, 5 (3): 190-192. 10.1038/nn0302-190.
Preuss TM: What is it like to be a human?. The Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by: Gazzaniga MS. 2004, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 5-22. Third
Rilling JK: Human and nonhuman primate brains: are they allometrically scaled versions of the same design?. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2006, 15 (2): 65-77. 10.1002/evan.20095.
Schoenemann PT, Sheehan MJ, Glotzer LD: Prefrontal white matter volume is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nat Neurosci. 2005, 8 (2): 242-252. 10.1038/nn1394.
Semendeferi K, Lu A, Schenker N, Damasio H: Humans and great apes share a large frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002, 5 (3): 272-276. 10.1038/nn814.
Gannon PJ, Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Braun AR: Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog. Science. 1998, 279 (5348): 220-222. 10.1126/science.279.5348.220.
Gannon PJ, Kheck NM, Braun AR, Holloway RL: Planum parietale of chimpanzees and orangutans: a comparative resonance of human-like planum temporale asymmetry. Anat Rec A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol. 2005, 287 (1): 1128-1141.
Hopkins WD, Marino L, Rilling JK, MacGregor LA: Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging(MRI). Neuroreport. 1998, 9 (12): 2913-2918. 10.1097/00001756-199808240-00043.
Hopkins WD: Chimpanzee right-handedness: internal and external validity in the assessment of hand use. Cortex. 2006, 42 (1): 90-93.
Annett M: Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory. 1985, London; Hilliside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Corballis MC: The Lopsided Ape: Evolution of the Generative Mind. 1991, New York: Oxford University Press
Crow TJ: Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: a resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews. 2000, 31 (2–3): 118-129. 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00029-6.
Preuss TM, Coleman GQ: Human-specific organization of primary visual cortex: alternating compartments of dense Cat-301 and calbindin immunoreactivity in layer 4A. Cereb Cortex. 2002, 12 (7): 671-691. 10.1093/cercor/12.7.671.
Hof PR, Nimchinsky EA, Perl DP, Erwin JM: An unusual population of pyramidal neurons in the anterior cingulate cortex of hominids contains the calcium-binding protein calretinin. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 307 (3): 139-142. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01964-4.
Nimchinsky EA, Gilissen E, Allman JM, Perl DP, Erwin JM, Hof PR: A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999, 96 (9): 5268-5273. 10.1073/pnas.96.9.5268.
Allman JM, Watson KK, Tetreault NA, Hakeem AY: Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005, 9 (8): 367-373. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.008.
Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, Litaker M, Roy E, Casanova MF: Lateralization of minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in nonhuman primate cortex. Brain Behav Evol. 2001, 57 (6): 349-358. 10.1159/000047253.
Orban GA, Van Essen D, Vanduffel W: Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004, 8 (7): 315-324. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.009.
Preuss TM: Specializations of the human visual system: The monkey model meets human reality. The Primate Visual System. Edited by: Kaas JH, Collins CE. 2004, Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 231-259.
Sereno MI, Tootell RB: From monkeys to humans: what do we now know about brain homologies?. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005, 15 (2): 135-144. 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.014.
Letinic K, Zoncu R, Rakic P: Origin of GABAergic neurons in the human neocortex. Nature. 2002, 417 (6889): 645-649. 10.1038/nature00779.
Logan CA: The altered rationale for the choice of a standard animal in experimental psychology: Henry H. Donaldson, Adolf Meyer and 'the' albino rat. History of Psychology. 1999, 2: 3-24. 10.1037/1093-4510.2.1.3.
Logan CA: Before there were standards: The role of test animals in the production of empirical generality in physiology. Journal of the History of Biology. 2002, 35 (2): 329-363. 10.1023/A:1016036223348.
Preuss TM: The argument from animals to humans in cognitive neuroscience. The Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by: Gazzaniga MS. 1995, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1227-1241. (Reprinted in Cognitive Neuroscience: A Reader, M.S. Gazzaniga, ed. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 2000).
Preuss TM: Taking the measure of diversity: comparative alternatives to the model-animal paradigm in cortical neuroscience. Brain Behav Evol. 2000, 55 (6): 287-299. 10.1159/000006664.
Rader KA: Making Mice: Standardizing Animals for American Biomedical Research, 1900–1955. 2004, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
DeFelipe J, Alonso-Nanclares L, Arellano JI: Microstructure of the neocortex: comparative aspects. J Neurocytol. 2002, 31 (3–5): 299-316. 10.1023/A:1024130211265.
Hof PR, Glezer , Nimchinsky EA, Erwin JM: Neurochemical and cellular specializations in the mammalian neocortex reflect phylogenetic relationships: evidence from primates, cetaceans, and artiodactyls. Brain Behav Evol. 2000, 55 (6): 300-310. 10.1159/000006665.
Skoglund TS, Pascher R, Berthold CH: Heterogeneity in the columnar number of neurons in different neocortical areas in the rat. Neurosci Lett. 1996, 208 (2): 97-100. 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12569-6.
Preuss TM: The discovery of cerebral diversity: An unwelcome scientific revolution. Evolutionary Anatomy of the Primate Cerebral Cortex. Edited by: Falk D, Gibson K. 2001, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 138-164.
Hammock EA, Young LJ: Microsatellite instability generates diversity in brain and sociobehavioral traits. Science. 2005, 308 (5728): 1630-1634. 10.1126/science.1111427.
Young LJ, Huot B, Nilsen R, Wang Z, Insel TR: Species differences in central oxytocin receptor gene expression: comparative analysis of promoter sequences. J Neuroendocrinol. 1996, 8 (10): 777-783. 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1996.05188.x.
Varki A: A chimpanzee genome project is a biomedical imperative. Genome Res. 2000, 10 (8): 1065-1070. 10.1101/gr.10.8.1065.
Walker LC, Cork LC: The neurobiology of aging in nonhuman primates. Alzheimer's Disease. Edited by: Terry RD, Katzman R, Bick KL, Sisodia SS. 1999, Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 233-243. 2
Denys K, Vanduffel W, Fize D, Nelissen K, Sawamura H, Georgieva S, Vogels R, Van Essen D, Orban GA: Visual activation in prefrontal cortex is stronger in monkeys than in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004, 16 (9): 1505-1516. 10.1162/0898929042568505.
Orban GA, Claeys K, Nelissen K, Smans R, Sunaert S, Todd JT, Wardak C, Durand JB, Vanduffel W: Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia. 2006, 44 (13): 2647-67. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.001.
Croxson PL, Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Pinsk MA, Gross CG, Richter W, Richter MC, Kastner S, Rushworth MF: Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human and macaque using probabilistic diffusion tractography. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (39): 8854-8866. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1311-05.2005.
Ramnani N, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Richter MC, Pinsk MA, Andersson JL, Rudebeck P, Ciccarelli O, Richter W, Thompson AJ, et al: The evolution of prefrontal inputs to the cortico-pontine system: diffusion imaging evidence from Macaque monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2006, 16 (6): 811-818. 10.1093/cercor/bhj024.
Barks SK, Rilling JK, Parr L, Preuss TM, Bremner JD, Votaw JW: Comparing resting brain activity in monkeys, apes and humans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2005, 39: (abstract).
Rilling JK, Glasser MF, Preuss TM, Ma X, Zhang Z, Zhao T, Hu X, Behrens TEJ: A comparative diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study of the arcuate fasciculus language pathway in humans, chimpanzees and rhesus macaques. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2006,
Brodmann K: Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrhinde. Edited by: Garey LJ. 1909, London: Smith-Gordon, Leipzig: Barth (reprinted as Brodmann's 'Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex, 1994)
Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Morosan P, Eickhoff SB, Kowalski T, de Vos K, Amunts K, Zilles K: Quantitative architectural analysis: a new approach to cortical mapping. Anat Embryol (Berl). 2005, 210 (5–6): 373-386. 10.1007/s00429-005-0028-2.
Sherwood CC, Raghanti MA, Simpson CD, Bonar CJ, de Sousa AA, Preuss TM, Hof PR: Scaling of inhibitory interneurons in areas V1 and V2 of anthropoid primates as revealed by calcium-binding protein immunohistochemistry. Brain Behav Evol. 2006, 69 (3): 176-95. 10.1159/000096986.
Carroll SB: Genetics and the making of Homo sapiens. Nature. 2003, 422 (6934): 849-857. 10.1038/nature01495.
Preuss TM, Cáceres M, Oldham MC, Geschwind DH: Human brain evolution: insights from microarrays. Nat Rev Genet. 2004, 5 (11): 850-860. 10.1038/nrg1469.
Ruvolo M: Comparative primate genomics: the year of the chimpanzee. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2004, 14 (6): 650-656. 10.1016/j.gde.2004.08.007.
Antonell A, de Luis O, Domingo-Roura X, Perez-Jurado LA: Evolutionary mechanisms shaping the genomic structure of the Williams-Beuren syndrome chromosomal region at human 7qll.23. Genome Res. 2005, 15 (9): 1179-1188. 10.1101/gr.3944605.
Eichler EE, Johnson ME, Alkan C, Tuzun E, Sahinalp C, Misceo D, Archidiacono N, Rocchi M: Divergent origins and concerted expansion of two segmental duplications on chromosome 16. J Hered. 2001, 92 (6): 462-468. 10.1093/jhered/92.6.462.
Hayakawa T, Angata T, Lewis AL, Mikkelsen TS, Varki NM, Varki A: A human-specific gene in microglia. Science. 2005, 309 (5741): 1693-
Varki A, Wills C, Perlmutter D, Woodruff D, Gage F, Moore J, Semendeferi K, Bernirschke K, Katzman R, Doolittle R, et al: Great ape phenome project?. Science. 1998, 282 (5387): 239-240. 10.1126/science.282.5387.239d.
Gagneux P: A Pan-oramic view: insights into hominoid evolution through the chimpanzee genome. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2004, 19 (11): 571-576. 10.1016/j.tree.2004.09.002.
Cáceres M, Suwyn C, Maddox M, Thomas JW, Preuss TM: Increased cortical expression of two synaptogenic thrombospondins in human brain evolution. Cerebral Cortex. 2007,
Acknowledgements
The author is supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundation (JSMF 21002093), the Yerkes National Primate Research Center under NRCC grant RR00165, and the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience under the STC program of the National Science Foundation (IBN-9876754).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
TMP is the sole author of this paper.
Rights and permissions
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
About this article
Cite this article
Preuss, T.M. Who's afraid of Homo sapiens?. J Biomed Discov Collaboration 1, 17 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-5333-1-17
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-5333-1-17